Volume 3, pages 277-288
©1977 Alan R. Liss, Inc.
The Medial Amygdala: Serotonergic Inhibition of Shock-Induced Aggression and Pain Sensitivity
in Rats
R. J.
Rodgers
Postgraduate
School of Studies in Psychology. University of Bradford.
Bradford,
England
Abstract
Two aspects
of the amygdaloid complex (corticomedial and basolateral) were examinedwitli
reference to serotonergic inliibition of shock-induced aggression.
Fighting
was significantly depressed by serotonergic stimulation (5-MT, 10 u% bilateral)in
the cortieomedial amygdala while serotonergic blockade (metliysergide,5 tigbilateral)
in this region increased levels of fighting. No consistent effects wereobtained
with serotonergic manipulation of the basolateral amygdala.
Further
investigation revealed that the stale of serotonergic activity in
medialamygdaloid sites was associated with concomitant alterations in the
animals'sensitivity to footshock. Results are discussed in relation to a) a
general inhibitoryrole of serotonin irbehavioural mechanisms and b) a
dopaminergic-serotonergicbalance for behavioural arousal involving medial
amygdaloidnudei.
Key words:
rat, shock-induced aggression, pain sensitivity, amygdala, serotonin,
inhibition
INTRODUCTION
In recent years,
central serotonergic (5-HT) mechanisms have been implicatedin the inhibitory
control of various fomis of aggressive behaviour. Reductions ofserotonergic
activity by PCPA injection have been reported to increase inter-malefighting in
mice (Essman et al, 1974), irritability (Koe and Weissman, 1966) andhome-cage
aggression (Sheard, 1973) in rats, and predatory attack in both cats(Ferguson
et al, 1970)and rats(DiChiaraet al, 1971; Eichelman and Thoa, 1973;Manuscript
received January 20, 1977; accepted February 9. 1977Address reprint requests to
R. J. Rodgers, Postgraduate School of Studies in PsychologyKarli et al. 1969;
McLain et al, 1974; Miczek et al. 1975; Vergnes. Mack et al,1974b).
Similarly, lesions of the serotonin-containing cell bodies in the raphe
systemincrease home-cage aggression (Sheard, 1973) and muricidebehaviour
(Grantet al, 1973; Vergnes, Mack et al, 1974b; Vergnes, Boehrer et al. i974a)
in rats.Conversely, increased serotonergic activity by injection of the 5-HT
precursor,dl-5HTP, inliibits isolated-induced fighting in mice (Hodge and
Butcher, 1974)and rat muricidalbehaviour (DiChiaraetal. 1971; Kulkarni, 1968 and 1970;Miczek et al, 1975;
Vergnes, Mack et al, 1974b).
Less
consistent results have been obtained in studies on shock-induced
aggression(SIA). Depletion of brain serotonin by PCPA has been reported to
increase(Butcher and Dietrich, 1974; Ellison and Bresler. 1974), have no effect
on (Conneret al, 1970; Eichelman and Thoa, 1973; McLain et al, 1974) or to
decrease(Anandet al, 1976) levels of SIA. Lesionsof the dorsal, but not the
medial, raphenuclei increase SIA by 83% over control levels of fighting (Jacobs
and Cohen, 1976).
The
amygdaloid complex has long been known to exert an importantmodulatory
influence upon aggressive behaviour in many species (rat: Eichelman,1971;
hamster: Bunnell et al, 1970; cat: Kaada, 1969; monkey: Rosvoldetal,1957; man:
Mark and Ervin. 1970). Most of these reports have suggested that theamygdala
facilitates aggressive responding, with lesions resulting in a
"tamingeffect." However, data exists indicating that the amygdala may
also exett aninhibitory influence over certain forms of aggressive behaviour,
lesions resultingin increased aggression (Bard and Mountcastle, 1948; Schreiner
and Kling, 1953;Utsin, 1965; Wood, 1958). This apparent confusion may be
explained by stressingthe fact that the amygdala is a nonhomogenous structure,
consisting of a complexof nuclei with differing afferent and efferent
connections (Hall. 1972; Lammers.1972;deOlinos. 1972).
Earlier
reports have implicated cholinergic facilitation of SAI in the
basolateralaspect of the amygdaloid complex (Rodgers and Brown. 1976). Since
evidenceexists for serotonergic terminals in both aspects of the amygdaloid
complex(Aghajanian, 1976. Eidelberg et al. 1967; Saavedra et al, 1974).the
present paperinvestigates the possibility that 5-HT may exert its inhibitory
effect upon SIAthrough this complex sub-cortical structure. Indeed, two
procedures known to increase muricidebehaviour in the rat, olfactory bulbectomy
and raphe lesions,decrease serotonin in the amygdaloid area (Karli et al, 1969;
Vergnes. Mack et al,1974b), perhaps reflecting decreased inhibition of
aggressive responding.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals
Adult male
Sprague-Dawley rats, ftom Bradford University colony, were usedin this study.
Subjects were individually housed with food and water available adlibitum. All
animals were maintained on a 12-hr light-dark cycle and testing was performed
under red light during the dark phase of the cycle, i.e., 7 p.m. onwards.
Surgery
Subjects
were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulae under Equithesinanaesthesia
(Jensen-Salsbery Lab., Inc.). Cannulae consisted of a 0.6-mm
o.d.stainless-steel guide, fitted with a 0.3-mm o.d. stylet. During injection,
styletswere replaced by 0.3-mm injection cannulae. Guides were constructed such
thateach terminated 2 mm dorsal to the desired injection site; injection
cannulae weredesigned to protrude 2 mm beyond the tip of the guides. After
surgery, eachanimal received an intramuscular injection of penicillin (50.000
units) andallowed at least 10 days postoperative recovery.
Stereotaxic Coordinates
Implantation
co-ordinates were based on adjusted calculations derived fromthe atlas on Konig
and Klippel (1963): Basolateral amygdala (+5.2, ± 5.0.8.5down) and
corticomedial amygdala (+5.0, ± 4.0,9.5 down). Anterior-posteriorvalues were
calculated with reference to stereotaxic zero.
Apparatus
A modified
rat operant station (manipulanda removed, flat interface). Measuring 23.5 X 22
X 22 cm served as the test chamber for both the aggressionand pain sensitivity
tests. The chamber was opaque apart from the perspex doorwhich also served as
an observation window. An Aim Biosciences shock generator(model 507) and
programming equipment supplied scrambled electric shock ofspecified intensity,
duration, and frequency to the grid floor of the test chamber.
Drugs
Serotonin
creatininesulphate complex (Sigma Chemical Co.) was used in asolution of 10
mg/ml. Methysergidebimaleate (Sandoz Products Ltd.) was usedin a solution of 5
mg/ml. Serotonin was dissolved in distilled water. Distilled waterwas used as
the control injection. To aid the solubility of methysergide, 1 dropof 5 N HCl
was added to the distilled water, a similar solution being used forthe control
injection.
Histology
Before
sacrifice, animals received bilateral injection of 0.1 /iltrypan blue toaid
localization of injection sites. Animals then received an overdose of
Nembutaland were perfused with physiological saline followed by 10% formal
saline. Brainswere removed, hardened in formalin, and examined for injection
sites followingsectioning on a freeze microtome.
EXPERIMENT 1 - SHOCK INDUCED AGGRESSION
Subjects
Twenty rats
were randomly divided into two equal groups. One group servedas unoperated
fight opponents while the other group was subdivided into twooperation groups.
Group I (n = 5) was bilaterally implanted with guide cannulaeaimed at the
basolateral amygdala (BLA) and Group II (n = 5) was implanted inthe
corticomedial amygdala (CMA).
Procedure
Operated
animals and unoperated fight opponents were matched for weightand fighting
pairs placed in the test chamber where they were allowed 5 min
explorationbefore testing commenced. Sixty pre-injection shocks (2 mA
intensity;0.5 sec duration, 6/min) were delivered to the grid fioor in order to
establishbaseline levels of responding. The responses were recorded as a) no
response,b) upright threat posture, and c) fight/attack response. An attack
response was recorded when one animal made a directed forward movement towards
anopponent either by lunging with its whole body or by striking with the
forepaws.
On four separate occasions (48 hr intersession interval) baselines
wereestablished for each fighting pair, after which they received 1 {A bilateral
injectionof either serotonin (10 jUg), serotonin vehicle, methysergide (5 )jg),
or methysergidevehicle and were immediately retested with a further 60 shocks.
All treatmentswere administered in a randomized sequence.
Results
Data was
analysed using 2-tailed correlated t tests, comparing baseline againstresponse.
Had any consistent effect upon levels of SIA, when injected into the BLA
animals.However, when injected into the CMA, serotonin produced a 40% decrease
in SIA(p < 0.001), while vehicle was without effect. Conversely,
methysergide injectioninto the CMA produced a 46% increase in levels of SIA (p
< 0.01). Again. Vehicle was without effect.
Figure 2
gives histological summaries of the area covered by injection sites ineach of
the two amygdala groups. Although one set of CMA injections were veryclose to
the ventral surface of the brain, the results were in the same direction asthe
rest of the group and were included in the data analysis.The results of this
experiment indicate that serotonergic inhibition of SIAoperates through a
forebrain system involving the corticomedial, but not thebasolateral, amygdala.
However, since changes in 5-HT in the forebrain have beenlinked to alterations
in pain sensitivity (Lints and Harvey, 1969; Yunger andHarvey, 1973), it was
considered appropriate to perform another experiment to test the possibility
that the present effects on aggression were merely secondaryto changes in pain
sensitivity.
CORTICOMEDIAL BASOLATERAL
AMYGDALA SITE
Fig, 1. Mean
attack frequencies {t standard deviation) for injected animals under various
surgical
and drug conditions. B) baseline, S) serotonin (10 /igbilat), M) methysergide
(5 Hg
bilat), V) appropriate vehicle control injection.
EXPERIMENT 2 - PAIISI SENSITIVITY
Subjects and Procedure
Five naive
rats were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulae aimed at thecorticomediai
amygdala.Pain thresholds were measured using a modified "flinch-jump"
threshold test(Evans, 1961; Rodgers and Brown, 1976). Animals were placed
individually inthe test chamber and received 6 series of 8 shocks (0.5 sec
duration) delivered at10 sec intervals to the grid floor. Shock series were
administered in ascendingand descending order with intensities ranging betweeti
0.16-1.3 mA in eiglitsteps. Only jump thresholds (the intensity at which the
animals' hind feet leavethe grid floor) were recorded since it has been
suggested that flinch thresholdsare unreUable indicators of pain sensitivity
(Evans, 1961). Thresholds weredetermined for each shock series and an overall
mean value calculated to yield a pre-injection basehne.
Ajiimals then received bilateral injections of either serotonin (10 jUg),
orserotonin vehicle, methysergide (5 /ig), or methysergide vehicle and
wereimmediately retested. Each animal received all injections over a period of
10 days.All treatments were administered in a randomized sequence.
Results
Data was
analyzed using 1-tailed correlated t tests, comparing baseUnesagainst
responses.Injections of serotonin into the CMA resulted in a significant
elevation in jumpthresholds (Baseline 0.81 ± 0.04 mA/Response 0.87 ± 0.05 mA; t
=-8.25, df = 4,p < 0.005) whereas vehicle was without effect (Baseline 0.80
± 0.04 mA/Response 0-82 ±0,03 mA; t =-1.31, n.s.).Conversely, methysergide
injection into CMA produced a significant decreasein jump thresholds (Baseline
0.77 ±0.05 mA/Response 0.69 ±0.05 mA; t =2.81,df = 4, p < 0.05) while
vehicle was ineffective (Baseline 0.77 ± 0.04 mA/Response0.78 ± 0.04 mA;t
=-1.96, n.s.).Histological examination indicated that injection sites were
indistinguishablefrom those reported in Experiment 1 (Eig. 2). Results from this
experiment strongly suggest that serotonergic manipulationof the amygdala ahers
aggression indirectly via changes in the animals sensitivityto electric shock.
DISCUSSION
This study
suggests that within the amygdaloid complex, 5-HT mediates aninhibitory influence
over aggressive responding and shock sensitivity. Although5-HT is equally
distributed in the amygdaloid nuclei (Saavedra et al., 1974),these particular
behavioural effects are seen specifically in relation to the
corticomediaiaspect of this complex. Present data are in agreement with earlier
findingsand related hypotheses concerning the role of 5-HT in other forms of
aggressivebehaviour: Vergnes, Boehrer et al (1974a) and Kulkarni (1968) have
independentlysuggested that the amygdala may be the critical focus for the
mediation ofserotonergic inhibition of muricide while Sheard (1969) proposed
the amygdalaas important in the increased irritability and aggressiveness seen
in rats followingPCPA injection. Further confirmation of serotonergic
inliibition in the amygdalahas been provided by the electrophysiological
studies of Eidelberg et al (1967)
andAghajanian
(1976).However, caution should be exercised with respect to the overall
interpretationof all these results. A major complicating factor in studies of serotonergic
inhibitionof aggression is that this transmitter system appears to exert a
general inliibitoryeffect upon behaviour and that its depletion leads to
heightened reactivity(commented upon at length by Vergnes, Boehrer et al,
1974a). Depletion of forebrain5-HT by medial forebrain bundle (MFB) lesions
{Harvey and Lints, 1965,1971; Lints and Harvey, 1969; Yunger and Harvey, 1973)
or PCPA injection(Harvey and Lints, 1971; Tenen, 1967, 1968) increases
responsiveness to painfulstimulation. Similarly, raphe lesions also increase
responsiveness to pain (Blasiget al, 1973; Proudfit and Anderson, 1975), but
reports also exist indicating noeffeet of such lesions on pain responses (Hole
and Lorens, 1975; Jacobs and Cohen,1976). Median raphe lesions increase open-field
activity and etnotionality (Jacobsand Cohen, 1976; Vergnes, Boehrer et al,
1974a).Evidence for central serotonergic inhibition of behaviour also derives
fromcentral injection studies. Intraventricular injection of 5-HT inhibits the
startleresponse to air puffs (Geyer et al, 1976), intrahypothalamic 5-HT
inhibitsaversive responding (LeRoux and Myers, 1975) and wliileintraseptal
5-HTinhibits pain sensitivity, injections of the 5-HT blocker, cinnanserin,
into thesame area increases pain sensitivity (Persip and Hamilton, 1973).
The results
of the present experiment appear to fit the general framework ofthese findings.
Serotonergic stimulation of the corti come dial amygdala inhibitsnot only
aggression but also pain sensitivity while serotonergic blockade(methysergide)
has the opposite effect.It has earlier been reported that dopaminergic
stimulation in the corticomedialamygdala facilitates SIA and pain sensitivity,
an effect perhaps related to generaldopaminergic facilitation of behavioural
arousal (Rodgers et al, 1976). Currentdata indicate serotonergic inhibition of
the same behaviours within the sameamygdaloid region. Together, these findings
may implicate the amygdala as acritical focus for the serotonergic inhibition
of catecholamine-induced arousal(Mabry and Campbell, 1973). In view of the
extensive sensory contribution tothe amygdala (Gloor, 1960; Goddard, 1964), it
may be the case that a "balance"between 5-HT and dopamine in the
corticomedial amygdala (dependent uponsensory input?) determines the threshold
for release of aggression by thecholinergic basolateral amygdala. This
infiuenee may be mediated to lower midbraineffector meehanisms(Bandler, 1973)
via the ventral amygdalofugal pathway,lateral hypothalamus, and associated
descending MFB connections, all of whichcontain cholinergic components
implieated in the control of aggression (seeRodgers and Brown, 1976).
Electrolytic lesions of all these structures decreaseaggression (Eichelman,
1971; Hilton and Zbrozyna, 1963; Karli et al, 1969;Miczek et al, 1974; Rodgers
and Brown, 1976; Ursin and Kaada, 1960a, b).Alternatively, the serotonergie
inhibitory influence tnay be mediated via themore indirect striaterminalis
route through the ventromedial hypothalamus tothe lateral hypothalamus. Lesions
of the striaterminalis (Mabry and Campbell,1975; Ursin, 1965) and ventromedial
hypothalamus (Adams, 1971; Eclancher andKarli, 1971; Eichelman, 1971; Grossman,
1972; Panksepp, 1971) are known to
increase
aggression, presumably through disinhibition. Present results do notallow the
selection of either alternative hypothesis. However, Bell
(personalcommunication) has found that serotonergic inhibition within the
lateral hypothalamusresults in increased levels of SIA and pain sensitivity,
while serotonergiestimulation decreases both of these behaviours. This would
indicate that thesecond alternative is the more likely explanation at the
moment.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author
wishes to thank Sandoz Products Ltd., for their kind gift of
!methysergidebimaleate. Thanks also to Mike Crowley, Dick Goundry, andPenny
Walker for invaluable technical and secretarial assistance during this study.
REFERENCES
Adams, D.
B. (1971).Defence and territorial behaviour dissociated by hypothalamiclesions
in the rat. Nature (London) 232:573-574.
Anand, M.,
Gupta, G. P., and Bhargava, K. P. (1976).Effect of tryptaminergicdrugs on
electroshock fighting behaviour in rats. Bur. J. Pharmacol. 39:389-391. I
Aghajanian,
G. K. (1976). LSD and 2-Bromo-LSD: Comparison of effeets onserotonergic
neurones and on neurones in two serotonergic projection areas,the ventral
lateral geniculate and amygdala. Neuropharmaeology 15:521-528.
Bandler, R.
J. (1973). Predatory aggression: Midbrain-pontine junction ratherthan
hypothalamus as the critical strueture? Aggress. Behav.1:261—267.
Bard, P.,
and Mountcastle, V. B. (1948). Some forebrain mechanisms involved inthe
expression of rage with special reference to suppression of angry
behaviour.Res. Pub. Assoc. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 27:262-402.
Blasig, J.,
Rheinhold, K., and Herz, A. (1973).Effeet of 6-hydroxydopamine 5,
6-dihydroxytryptamine and raphe lesion on the antinociceptive action ofmorphine
in rats.Psychopharmacologia (Berlin) 31:1 11-119.Bunnell, B. N., Sodetz, F. J.,
and Shalloway, D. 1.(1970). Amygdaloid lesions andsocial behaviour in the
golden hamster. Physiol. Behav. 5:153-161.
Butcher, L.
L., and Dietrich, A. P. (1973).Effects on shock-elicited aggressionin mice of
preferentially protecting brain monoamines against the depletingaction of reserpine.Nauyn-Schmiedeb.
Arch. Pharmacol. 277:61.
Conner, R.
L., Stolk, J. M., Barchas, J. D., Dement, W. C, and Levine, S. (1970).Effects
of PCPA on shock-induced fighting behaviour in rats. Physiol. Behav5:1221-1224.
DiChiara,
G., Camba, R., and Spano, P. F. (1971).Evidence for inhibition bybrain
serotonin of mouse-killing behaviour in rats. Nature (London)233:272-273.
Eclancher,
F., and Karli, P. (1971). Comportment d'agressioninterspeeifiqueet comportment
alimentaire du Rat: effets de lesions des noyauxventromediansde l'hypothalamus.
Brain Res. 26:71-79.
Eiehelman,
B, S. (1971). Effect of subcortica! lesions on shock-induced aggressionin the
rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol 74:331-339.286 Rodgers
Eichelman,
B. S., and Thoa, N. B. (1973).The aggressive monoamines. Biol.Psychiat.
6:143-164.
Eidelberg,
E., Goldstein, G. P., and Deza, L. (1967).Evidence for serotonin as apossible
inhibitory transmitter in some limbic structures. Exp. Brain. Res,4:73-80.
Ellison, G.
D., and Bresler, D. E. (1974).Tests of emotional behaviour in ratsfollowing
depletion of norepinephrine, of serotonin, or of
both.Psyehopharmacologia(Berlin) 34:275-288.
Essman, W.
B., Kostowski, W., and Valzelli, L. (1974).Alterations in aggressivebehaviour
in miee with changes in brain 5-hydroxytryptamine regulation.Fed.Proc. 33:466.
Evans, W.
O. (1961). A new technique for the investigation of some analgesicdrugs on a
reflexive behaviour in the rat. Psychopharmacologia (Berlin)2:318-325.
Ferguson,
J., Henriksen, S., Cohen, J., Barchas, J., and Dement, W.
(1970)."Hypersexuahty" and behavioural changes in cats caused by
administration ofp-chloraphenylalanine. Science 168:499—501,
Geyer, M.
A., Warbritton, J. D., Menkes, D. B., Zook, J. A., and Mandell, A. J.(1975).
Opposite effects of intraventricular serotonin and bufotenin on ratstartle
responses. Pharm. Bioehem. Behav, 3:687- 691.
Gloor, P.
(1960). Amygdala. In Handbook of Physiology, Section 1: NeurophysiologyVol. 2.
Washington: Ameriean Physiological Society, pp. 1395-1421.
Goddard,G.
V. (1964). Functions of the amygdala. Psych. Bull. 62:89-109.Grant, L. D.,
Coscina, D. V., Grossman, S. P., and Freedman, D. X. (1973).
Muricide
after serotonin-depleting lesions of the midbrain raphe nuclei. Pharm.Biochem.
Behav. 1:77-80.
Grossman,
S. P. (1972). Aggression, avoidance and reaction to novel environmentsiji
female rats with ventromedial hypothalamie lesions, J. Comp. Physiol.Psychoi.
78:274-283.
Hali, E.
(1972). Some aspects of the structural organization of the amygdala.
In"The Neurobiology of the Amygdala," Eleftheriou B. E. (ed.). New
York:Plenum Press, pp. 95-123.
Harvey, J.
A., and Lints, C. E. (1965).Eesions of the MFB: delayed effects onsensitivity
to footshock. Science 148:250-252.
Harvey, J.
A., and Lints, C. E. (1971). Lesions of the medial forebrain
bundle:relationship between pain sensitivity and telencephalic content of
serotonin.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychoi. 74:28-36.
Hilton, S.
M., and Zbrozyna, A. W. (1963).Amygdaloid region for defence reactionsand its
efferent pathway to the brain stem. J. Physiol. 165:160-173.
Hodge, G.
K., and Butcher, L. L. (1974). 5-Hydroxytryptamine correlates
ofisolation-induced aggression in mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 28:326-337.
Hole, K.,
and Lorens, S. A. (1975). Response to electric shoek in rats: Effects
ofselective midbrain raphe lesions. Pharm. Biochetn. Behav. 3:95-102.
Jacobs, B.
L., and Cohen, A. (1976). Differential behavioural effects of lesions ofthe
median or dorsal raphe nuelei in rats: Open field and pain-elicited aggression.
J. Comp. Physiol.
Psychoi. 90:102-108.KarIi, P., Vergnes, M,, and Didiergeorges, F. (1969). Rat-mouse interspecific aggressive
behaviour and its manipulation by brain ablation and by brainstimulation. In
"Aggressive Behaviour," Garattini, S., and Sigg, E, B. (eds.). 'New
York: Wiley, pp. 47-55.
Koe.B,, and
Weissman, A, (1966). P-chlorophenylalanine: a specific depletor ofbrain
serotonin. J. Pharm. Exp. Ther. 154:499-516.
Konig, J.
F. R., and Klippel, R. A. (1963).'The Rat Brain - A Stereotaxic
Atlas."Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Kulkarni, A.
S. (1968). Murieidal block produced by 5-hydroxytryptophan and 'various drugs.
Life Sci, 7:125-128. '
Kulkarni,
A. S. (1970). Deearboxylase inhibitor on 5-HTP-induced blockade of
Imouse-killing. Pharmacologist 12:207.
Lammers, H,
J. (1972).The neural connections oftheamygdaloid complex inmammals. In 'The
Neurobiology of the Amygdala," Eleftheriou, B. E. (ed.), I New York:
Plenum Press, pp. 123-145.
Le Roux, A.
G., and Myers, R. D. (1975). Action of serotonin microinjected intohypothalamic
sites at whieh electrical stimulation produced aversive responsesPhysiol.
Behav. 14:501-505.
Lints, C.
E., and Harvey, J. A. (1969).Drug-induced reversal of brain damage inthe
rat.Physiot, Behav. 4:29-3 1.
Mabry, P,
D., and Campbell, B. A. (1973).Serotonergic inhibition of
catecholamineindueedbehavioural arousal. Brain Res. 49:381-391.
Mabry, P.
D., and Campbell, B. A. (1975). Food deprivation-induced behaviouralarousal:
Mediation by hypothalamus and amygdala. J. Comp. PhysiolPsychol89:19-38.
Kaada, B.
R. (1967). Brain mechanisms related to aggressive behaviour. In
"'Aggressionand Defense.Neural Mechanisms and Social
Patterns."ClemeKarIi, P., Vergnes, M,, and Didiergeorges, F. (1969).
Rat-mouse interspecific aggressive behaviour and its manipulation by brain
ablation and by brainstimulation. In "Aggressive Behaviour,"
Garattini, S., and Sigg, E, B. (eds.). 'New York: Wiley, pp. 47-55.
Koe.B,, and
Weissman, A, (1966). P-chlorophenylalanine: a specific depletor ofbrain
serotonin. J. Pharm. Exp. Ther. 154:499-516.
Konig, J.
F. R., and Klippel, R. A. (1963).'The Rat Brain - A Stereotaxic
Atlas."Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Kulkarni,
A. S. (1968). Murieidal block produced by 5-hydroxytryptophan and 'various
drugs. Life Sci, 7:125-128. '
Kulkarni,
A. S. (1970). Deearboxylase inhibitor on 5-HTP-induced blockade of
Imouse-killing. Pharmacologist 12:207.
Lammers, H,
J. (1972).The neural connections oftheamygdaloid complex inmammals. In 'The
Neurobiology of the Amygdala," Eleftheriou, B. E. (ed.), iNew York: Plenum
Press, pp. 123-145.
Le Roux, A.
G., and Myers, R. D. (1975). Action of serotonin microinjected intohypothalamic
sites at whieh electrical stimulation produced aversive responsesPhysiol.
Behav. 14:501-505.
Lints, C.
E., and Harvey, J. A. (1969).Drug-induced reversal of brain damage inthe
rat.Physiot, Behav. 4:29-3 1.
Mabry, P,
D., and Campbell, B. A. (1973).Serotonergic inhibition of
catecholamineindueedbehavioural arousal. Brain Res. 49:381-391.
Mabry, P.
D., and Campbell, B. A. (1975). Food deprivation-induced behaviouralarousal:
Mediation by hypothalamus and amygdala. J. Comp. PhysiolPsychol89:19-38.nte, C.
D.,and Lindsley,D. B. (eds.). Berkeley, Los Angeles: UCLA Press, pp. 95-133.
Rodgers, R.
J., Semple, J. M., Cooper, S. J., and Brown, K. (1976). Shock-inducedaggression
and pain sensitivity in the rat: Catecholamine involvement in thecorticomediai
amygdala. Aggress. Behav. 2:193-204.
Rosvold, H.
E., Mirsky, A. F., and Pribram, K. H. (1954). Influence ofamygdalectomy on
social behaviour in monkeys. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.47:173-178.
Saavedra,
J. M., Brownstein, M., and Palkovits, M. (1974).Serotonin distributionin the
limbic system of the rat. Brain Res. 79:437-441.
Schieiner,
L., and Kling, A. (1953).Behaviour changes following rhinencephalicinjury in
the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 16:643-658.
Sheard, M.
H. (1969). The effect of p-chlorophenylalanine on behaviour in rats:relation to
brain serotonin and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid. Brain Res.15:524-528.
Sheard, M.
H. (1973). Aggressive behaviour: Modification by
amphetamine,p-chlorophenylalanine and lithium in rats. Aggressologie
14:323-326.
Tenen, S.
S. (1967). The effects of PCPA, a serotonin depletor, on avoidance
acquisition,
pain sensitivity and related behaviours in the rat.
Psychopharmacologia
(Berlin) 10:204 -219.
Tenen, S. S.
(1968). Antagonism of
the analgesic effect of morphine and other
drugs by
p-chlorophenylalanine, a serotonin depletor. Psychopharmacologia
(BerUn)
12:278-285.
Ursin, H.
(1965). Effect of amygdaloid lesions on avoidance and visual discriminationin
cats. Exp. Neurol. 11:298-317.
Ursin, H.,
and Kaada, B. R. (1960a).Functional localization within the amygdaloidcomplex
of the cat. EEC Clin. Neurophysiol.12:1—20.
Ursin, H.,
and Kaada, B. R. (1960b).Subcortieal structures mediating the attentionresponse
induced by amygdala stimulation. Exp. Neurol. 2:109-122.
Vergnes, M.,
Boehrer, A., and Karli, P. (1974a). Interspecific aggressiveness andreactivity in mouse-killing and
non-killing rats: Compared effects of olfactorybulb removal and raphe lesions.
Aggress. Behav.1:1 — 16.
_____________________
BIBLIOGRAFIA
Rodgers, R.
(1977). The Medial Amygdala: Serotonergic
Inhibition of Shock-Induced Aggression and Pain Sensitivity in Rats. Liss, Inc. Volume 3, pages
277-288. Bradford, England
A pesar del año de publicación de este articulo (1977) representa un gran fundamento para las investigaciones posteriores desde un ámbito neuro científico, ya que ofrece una conclusión y una mirada distinta sobre la agresión y la sensibilidad al dolor.
ResponderEliminar